Logical-mathematical game in the context of early education. A study about the Romanian preschool teachers

CONSTANTIN PETROVICI, GEANINA HAVÂRNEANU

Abstract

In this paper, we intend to provide explanations for the fact that logical-mathematical games (a teaching method in which simulated procedures prevail and its goals are to build the capacity to understand the causal relations between actions and events, to develop logical reasoning, and to perform operations and logical deductions) have a lower recurrence in current educational activities, although their importance is well known. We have studied the hypothesis of a dependence between experience and use of the logical-mathematical game in daily teaching activities, the hypothesis that the teacher who has already used logical-mathematical games is more flexible in adapting a teaching game to become a logical-mathematical game, the hypothesis of teachers who have already used logical-mathematical games being more willing to apply this type of game to a wider variety of target groups, the hypothesis that there is a  direct connection between a teacher’s past experience with logical-mathematical games and his/her willingness to use a variety of resources and high professional standards in planning them. The research provides statistically significant evidence suggesting that only teachers with less experience/seniority in education appear more interested in using logical-mathematical games in daily teaching activities, and teachers who have experience with logical-mathematical games rely more on that very experience in planning teaching than on specialist reference. Additionally, educators with little interest in teaching mathematics itself had low capacity to do exercises that involve a high effort in teaching and limited willingness to conceive, design and implement complex projects.

Keywords

Logical-mathematical games, logical reasoning, operations and logical deductions, recurrence of logical-mathematical games in daily educational activities

Full Text:

PDF

References

Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Lekkanen, M.-L., & Nurni, J.-E. (2004). Developmental dynamics of the math performance from pre-school to Grade 2. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 699-713.

Barbu, H., Popescu E., & Şerban, F. (1993). Recreative and entertaining games. Bucureşti: Didactical and Pedagogical Publisher.

Bocoş, M. (2002). Interactive learning. Cluj: University Press.

Carroll, L. (1897). Symbolic Logic. London: Macmillan and Co.

Chateau J. (1970). The child and the game. Bucureşti: Didactical and Pedagogical Publisher.

Claparède, E. (1946). Psychologie de l’enfant et pédagogie expérimentale. 1. Le Développement mental. Neuchâtel, Paris: Delachaux Niestlé.

Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., Spitler, M. E., Lange, A. A., & Wolfe, C. B. (2011). Mathematics learned by young children in an intervention based on learning trajectories: a large- scale cluster randomized trial. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 42, 127-166.

De Singly, F., Blanchet, A., Gotman, A., & Kaufmann, J.-C. (1992). L’Enquête et ses méthodes. Paris: Éditions Nathan.

Diènes, Z. P., & Golding, W. E. (1970). Les premiers pas en mathématique. Logique et jeux logiques. Paris: Éditions O.C.D.L.

Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., et. al. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428-1446.

Fischbein, E. (1970). Diary of modern pedagogy, vol. 1. Bucureşti.

Galperin, P. J., & Kotik, N. R. (1982). To the psychology of creative thinking. Questions of psychology, 3.

Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nuget, L., & Bailey, D. H. (2013). Adolescents’ functional numeracy is predicted by their school entry number system knowledge. PLoS ONE, 8, e54651.

Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early math matters: kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45, 850-867.

Kagan, S. L., Moore, E., & Bredekamp, S. (Eds) (1995). Reconsidering children’s early learning and development: toward common views and vocabulary. Report of the National Education Goals Panel, Goal 1 Technical Planning Group. ERIC, ED391576. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

Li, X., Chi, L., DeBey, M., & Baroody, A. (2015). A study of early childhood mathematics teaching in the United States and China. Early Education and Development, 26(3), 450-478.

Petrovici, C. (2014). Didactics of mathematical activities in preschool. Iasi: Polirom Publisher.

Schulman-Kolombus, E. (1998). Preschool Didactics. Bucureşti: Editura V & Integral.

Seefeldt, C., & Galper, A. (2004). Mathematics - Study and teaching (Early childhood) - Activity programs. Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson/Merrill.

Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27, 623-656.

Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board games – but not circular ones – improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 545-560.

Siegler, R. S., Duncan, G. J., Davis-Kean, P. E., Duckworth, K., Claessens, A., Engel, M., et al. (2012). Early predictors of High School mathematics achievement. Psychological Science, 23, 691-697.

Thurstone, L. L. (1974). The measurement of values. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.


DOI: https://doi.org/10.26220/rev.2939

View Counter: Abstract | 448 | times, and PDF | 255 | times



Re S M ICT E | ISSN: 1792-3999 (electronic), 1791-261X (print) | Laboratory of Didactics of Sciences, Mathematics and ICT, Department of Educational Sciences and Early Childhood Education - University of Patras.

Pasithee | Library & Information Center | University of Patras